Skip to contents

1 Task description

In research on visual working memory participants are often asked to remember and reproduce continuous features of visual objects such as their color or orientation. In the continuous reproduction task (sometimes also called delayed estimation task), participants encode a set of visual objects into visual working memory and are then asked to reproduce a specific feature of one cued object on a continuous scale at test (see Figure 1.1 for an illustration).

Most often the features used in these tasks are colors sampled from a color wheel (Wilken and Ma 2004) or continuous orientations of a bar or a triangle (Bays et al. 2011). The set of to-be-remembered objects typically consists of one up to eight objects spatially distributed over the screen. Thus, participants must associate the to-be-remembered features (e.g. color or orientation) with the spatial locations they are presented at. The precision of the representation of an object’s feature in visual working memory is measured as the angular deviation from the true feature presented at encoding.

A typical continuous reproduction task

Figure 1.1: A typical continuous reproduction task

2 The role of measurement models

In these continuous reproduction tasks, the simplest measure of performance is the average angle deviation of the response from the true feature value. In many studies, this average recall error has been the main dependent variable for evaluating the effect of experimental manipulations. Yet, the average recall error confounds different properties of memory representations and does not sufficiently represent the theoretical processes assumed by current models of visual working memory. Therefore, different measurement models have been proposed to formalize distinct aspects of visual working memory models and how they translate into observed behavior.

A measurement model is a statistical model that describes the relationship between latent cognitive processes and observed behavior. For continuous reproduction tasks, measurement models provide a more refined representation of memory processes because they decompose the average recall error into several theoretically meaningful parameters.

At the core of these models is the assumption that responses in continuous reproduction tasks can stem from different distributions depending on the continuous activation of different memory representation or the cognitive state a person is in at recall.

3 CRT Models in the bmm package

The bmm package implements several measurement models for analyzing continuous reproduction data:

The two-parameter mixture model (Zhang and Luck 2008)
The three-parameter mixture model (Bays, Catalao, and Husain 2009)
The Interference Measurement Model (Oberauer and Lin 2017)
The Signal Discrimination Model (SDM) by (Oberauer 2023)

References

Bays, Paul M., Raquel F. G. Catalao, and Masud Husain. 2009. “The Precision of Visual Working Memory Is Set by Allocation of a Shared Resource.” Journal of Vision 9 (10): 7–7. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.10.7.
Bays, Paul M., Nikos Gorgoraptis, Natalie Wee, Louise Marshall, and Masud Husain. 2011. “Temporal Dynamics of Encoding, Storage, and Reallocation of Visual Working Memory.” Journal of Vision 11 (10): 6–6.
Oberauer, Klaus. 2023. “Measurement Models for Visual Working Memory—a Factorial Model Comparison.” Psychological Review 130 (3): 841–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000328.
Oberauer, Klaus, and Hsuan-Yu Lin. 2017. “An Interference Model of Visual Working Memory.” Psychological Review 124 (1): 21–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000044.
Wilken, Patrick, and Wei Ji Ma. 2004. “A Detection Theory Account of Change Detection.” Journal of Vision 4 (12): 11–11.
Zhang, Weiwei, and Steven J. Luck. 2008. “Discrete Fixed-Resolution Representations in Visual Working Memory.” Nature 453 (7192): 233–35. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06860.